In the transcript below, we highlighted certain words or expressions and linked them to their meaning or explanation. We hope you enjoy this topic and we highly encourage you to discuss this topic in your class, with your teacher or with other English users.
I need to make a confession at the outset here. A little over 20 years ago I did something that I regret, something that I'm not particularly
proud of,
something that, in many ways, I wish
no one would ever know, but here I feel kind of obliged to reveal. (Laughter) In the late 1980s, in a moment of youthful indiscretion, I went to law school. (Laughter)
Now, in America law is a professional
degree:
you get your university degree, then
you go on to law school. And when I got to law school, I didn't do very well. To put it mildly, I didn't do very
well.
I, in fact, graduated in the part of
my law school class that made the top 90 percent
possible.
(Laughter) Thank you. I never practiced law a day in my
life;
I pretty much wasn't allowed to. (Laughter)
But today, against my better
judgment,
against the advice of my own wife, I want to try to dust off some of those legal skills -- what's left of those legal skills. I don't want to tell you a story. I want to make a case. I want to make a hard-headed,
evidence-based, dare I say lawyerly case, for rethinking how we run our
businesses.
So, ladies and gentlemen of the jury,
take a look at this. This is called the candle problem. Some of you might have seen this
before.
It's created in 1945 by a psychologist named Karl Duncker. Karl Duncker created this experiment that is used in a whole variety of
experiments in behavioral science. And here's how it works. Suppose I'm
the experimenter. I bring you into a room. I give you a
candle,
some thumbtacks and some matches. And I say to you, "Your job is to attach the candle to the wall so the wax doesn't drip onto the
table." Now what would you do?
Now many people begin trying to thumbtack the candle to the wall. Doesn't work. Somebody, some people -- and I saw somebody kind of make the motion over here -- some people have a great idea where they light the match, melt the side of the candle, try to adhere it to the wall. It's an awesome idea. Doesn't work. And eventually, after five or 10 minutes, most people figure out the solution, which you can see here. The key is to overcome what's called functional fixedness. You look at that box and you see it only as a receptacle for the tacks. But it can also have this other function, as a platform for the candle. The candle problem.
Now I want to tell you about an
experiment
using the candle problem, done by a scientist named Sam
Glucksberg,
who is now at Princeton University in
the U.S.
This
shows the power of incentives.
Here's what he did. He gathered his
participants.
And he said, "I'm going to time
you. How quickly you can solve this problem?" To one group he said, "I'm going to time you to
establish norms, averages for how long it typically
takes
someone to solve this sort of
problem."
To the second group he offered
rewards.
He said, "If you're in the top
25 percent of the fastest times, you get five dollars. If you're the fastest of everyone
we're testing here today, you get 20 dollars." Now this is several years ago.
Adjusted for inflation, it's a decent sum of money for a few
minutes of work. It's a nice motivator.
Question: How much faster did this group solve the problem? Answer: It took them, on average, three and a half minutes longer. Three and a half minutes longer. Now
this makes no sense right? I mean, I'm an American. I believe in
free markets.
That's not how it's supposed to work.
Right?
(Laughter) If you want people to perform better, you reward them. Right? Bonuses, commissions, their own
reality show.
Incentivize them. That's how business works. But that's not happening here. You've got an incentive designed to sharpen thinking and accelerate
creativity,
and it does just the opposite. It dulls thinking and blocks creativity.
And what's interesting about this
experiment is that it's not an aberration. This has been replicated over and over and over again, for nearly 40 years. These contingent motivators -- if you do this, then you get that -- work in some circumstances. But for a lot of tasks, they actually
either don't work or, often, they do harm. This is one of the most robust
findings
in social science, and also one of the most ignored.
I spent the last couple of years
looking at the science of human motivation, particularly the dynamics of extrinsic motivators and intrinsic motivators. And I'm telling you, it's not even
close.
If you look at the science, there is
a mismatch between what science knows and what
business does.
And what's alarming here is that our
business operating system --think of the set of assumptions and
protocols beneath our businesses, how we motivate people, how we apply
our human resources -- it's built entirely around these
extrinsic motivators, around carrots and sticks. That's actually fine for many kinds
of 20th century tasks. But for 21st century tasks, that mechanistic,
reward-and-punishment approach
doesn't work, often doesn't work, and
often does harm. Let me show you what I mean.
So Glucksberg did another experiment
similar to this where he presented the problem in a
slightly different way, like this up here. Okay? Attach the candle to the wall so the
wax doesn't drip onto the table. Same deal. You: we're
timing for norms. You: we're incentivizing. What happened this time? This time, the incentivized group kicked the other group's butt. Why? Because when the tacks are out
of the box,
it's pretty easy isn't it? (Laughter)
If-then rewards work really well for those sorts of tasks, where there is a simple set of rules
and a clear destination to go to. Rewards, by their very nature, narrow our focus, concentrate the
mind;
that's why they work in so many
cases.
And so, for tasks like this, a narrow focus, where you just see
the goal right there, zoom straight ahead to it, they work really well. But for the real candle problem, you don't want to be looking like
this.
The solution is not over here. The
solution is on the periphery. You want to be looking around. That reward actually narrows our
focus
and restricts our possibility.
Let me tell you why this is so
important.
In western Europe, in many parts of Asia, in North America, in Australia, white-collar workers are doing less
of
this kind of work, and more of this kind of work. That routine, rule-based, left-brain work -- certain kinds of accounting, certain
kinds of financial analysis, certain kinds of computer programming
--
has become fairly easy to outsource, fairly easy to automate. Software can do it faster. Low-cost providers around the world
can do it cheaper. So what really matters are the more right-brained creative, conceptual kinds of
abilities.
Think about your own work. Think about your own work. Are the problems that you face, or
even the problems we've been talking about here, are those kinds of problems -- do
they have a clear set of rules, and a single solution? No. The rules are mystifying. The solution, if it exists at all, is surprising and not obvious. Everybody in this room is dealing with their own version of the candle problem. And for candle problems of any kind, in any field, those if-then rewards, the things around which we've built
so many of our businesses, don't work.
Now, I mean it makes me crazy. And this is not - here's the thing. This is not a feeling. Okay? I'm a lawyer; I don't believe
in feelings.
This is not a philosophy. I'm an American; I don't believe in
philosophy.
(Laughter) This is a fact -- or, as we say in my hometown of
Washington, D.C., a true fact. (Laughter) (Applause) Let me give you an example of what I
mean.
Let me marshal the evidence here, because I'm not telling you a story,
I'm making a case.
Ladies and gentlemen of the jury, some evidence: Dan Ariely, one of the great
economists of our time, he and three colleagues, did a study
of some MIT students. They gave these MIT students a bunch
of games,
games that involved creativity, and motor skills, and concentration. And the offered them, for
performance,
three levels of rewards: small reward, medium reward, large
reward.
Okay? If you do really well you get
the large reward, on down. What happened? As long as the task
involved only mechanical skill bonuses worked as they would be
expected:
the higher the pay, the better the
performance.
Okay? But one the task called for even rudimentary cognitive skill, a larger reward led to poorer
performance.
Then they said, "Okay let's see if there's any cultural bias here. Lets go to Madurai, India and test
this."
Standard of living is lower. In Madurai, a reward that is modest
in North American standards, is more meaningful there. Same deal. A bunch of games, three levels of rewards. What happens? People offered the medium level of
rewards
did no better than people offered the
small rewards.
But this time, people offered the
highest rewards, they did the worst of all. In eight of the nine tasks we
examined across three experiments, higher incentives led to worse
performance.
Is this some kind of touchy-feely socialist conspiracy going on here? No. These are economists from MIT, from Carnegie Mellon, from the
University of Chicago. And do you know who sponsored this
research?
The Federal Reserve Bank of the
United States.
That's the American experience.
There is a mismatch between what science knows and what business does. And what worries me, as we stand here in the rubble of the economic collapse, is that too many organizations are making their decisions, their policies about talent and people, based on assumptions that are outdated, unexamined, and rooted more in folklore than in science. And if we really want to get out of this economic mess, and if we really want high performance on those definitional tasks of the 21st century, the solution is not to do more of the wrong things, to entice people with a sweeter carrot, or threaten them with a sharper stick. We need a whole new approach.
And the good news about all of this is that the scientists who've been studying motivation have given us this new approach. It's an approach built much more around intrinsic motivation. Around the desire to do things because they matter, because we like it, because they're interesting, because they are part of something important. And to my mind, that new operating system for our businesses revolves around three elements: autonomy, mastery and purpose. Autonomy: the urge to direct our own lives. Mastery: the desire to get better and better at something that matters. Purpose: the yearning to do what we do in the service of something larger than ourselves. These are the building blocks of an entirely new operating system for our businesses.
I want to talk today only about autonomy. In the 20th century, we came up with this idea of management. Management did not emanate from nature. Management is like -- it's not a tree, it's a television set. Okay? Somebody invented it. And it doesn't mean it's going to work forever. Management is great. Traditional notions of management are great if you want compliance. But if you want engagement, self-direction works better.
Let me give you some examples of some kind of radical notions of self-direction. What this means -- you don't see a lot of it, but you see the first stirrings of something really interesting going on, because what it means is paying people adequately and fairly, absolutely -- getting the issue of money off the table, and then giving people lots of autonomy. Let me give you some examples.
How many of you have heard of the company Atlassian? It looks like less than half.(Laughter) Atlassian is an Australian software company. And they do something incredibly cool. A few times a year they tell their engineers, "Go for the next 24 hours and work on anything you want, as long as it's not part of your regular job. Work on anything you want."So that engineers use this time to come up with a cool patch for code, come up with an elegant hack. Then they present all of the stuff that they've developed to their teammates, to the rest of the company, in this wild and wooly all-hands meeting at the end of the day. And then, being Australians, everybody has a beer.
They call them FedEx Days. Why? Because you have to deliver something overnight. It's pretty. It's not bad. It's a huge trademark violation, but it's pretty clever. (Laughter) That one day of intense autonomy has produced a whole array of software fixes that might never have existed.
And it's worked so well that Atlassian has taken it to the next level with 20 Percent Time --done, famously, at Google -- where engineers can work, spend 20 percent of their time working on anything they want. They have autonomy over their time, their task, their team, their technique. Okay? Radical amounts of autonomy. And at Google, as many of you know, about half of the new products in a typical year are birthed during that 20 Percent Time: things like Gmail, Orkut, Google News.
Let me give you an even more radical example of it: something called the Results Only Work Environment, the ROWE, created by two American consultants, in place in place at about a dozen companies around North America. In a ROWE people don't have schedules. They show up when they want. They don't have to be in the office at a certain time, or any time. They just have to get their work done. How they do it, when they do it, where they do it, is totally up to them. Meetings in these kinds of environments are optional.
What happens? Almost across the board, productivity goes up, worker engagement goes up, worker satisfaction goes up, turnover goes down. Autonomy, mastery and purpose, These are the building blocks of a new way of doing things. Now some of you might look at this and say, "Hmm, that sounds nice, but it's Utopian." And I say, "Nope. I have proof."
The mid-1990s, Microsoft started an encyclopedia called Encarta. They had deployed all the right incentives, all the right incentives. They paid professionals to write and edit thousands of articles. Well-compensated managers oversaw the whole thing to make sure it came in on budget and on time. A few years later another encyclopedia got started. Different model, right? Do it for fun. No one gets paid a cent, or a Euro or a Yen. Do it because you like to do it.
Now if you had, just 10 years ago, if you had gone to an economist, anywhere, and said, "Hey, I've got these two different models for creating an encyclopedia. If they went head to head, who would win?" 10 years ago you could not have found a single sober economist anywhere on planet Earth who would have predicted the Wikipedia model.
This is the titanic battle between these two approaches. This is the Ali-Frazier of motivation. Right? This is the Thrilla' in Manila. Alright? Intrinsic motivators versus extrinsic motivators. Autonomy, mastery and purpose, versus carrot and sticks. And who wins? Intrinsic motivation, autonomy, mastery and purpose, in a knockout. Let me wrap up.
There is a mismatch between what science knows and what business does. And here is what science knows. One: Those 20th century rewards, those motivators we think are a natural part of business, do work, but only in a surprisingly narrow band of circumstances. Two: Those if-then rewards often destroy creativity. Three: The secret to high performance isn't rewards and punishments, but that unseen intrinsic drive -- the drive to do things for their own sake. The drive to do things cause they matter.
And here's the best part. Here's the best part. We already know this. The science confirms what we know in our hearts. So, if we repair this mismatch between what science knows and what business does, if we bring our motivation, notions of motivation into the 21st century, if we get past this lazy, dangerous, ideology of carrots and sticks, we can strengthen our businesses, we can solve a lot of those candle problems, and maybe, maybe, maybe we can change the world. I rest my case. (Applause)